Photo credit: Ben Earwicker, Garrison Photography, Boise, ID www.garrisonphoto.org |
A remarkable
schism is developing between audiologists in the UK and the USA on the topic of
auditory processing disorder (APD) in children. In 2010, the American Academy
of Audiology published clinical practice guidelines for auditory processing disorder. In 2011, the British Society
of Audiology published a position statement on the same topic, which came to
rather different conclusions. This month a White Paper by the British Society
of Audiology appeared reaffirming their position alongside invited
commentaries.
So what is all
the fuss about? The argument centres on how to diagnose APD in children. Most
of the tests used in the USA to identify APD involve responding to speech. One
of the most widely-used assessments is the SCAN-C battery which has four
subtests:
In 2006, David
Moore, Director of the Medical Research Council’s Institute of Hearing Research
in Nottingham, created a stir when he published a paper arguing that APD
diagnosis should be based on performance on non-linguistic tests of auditory
perception. Moore’s concern was that tests such as SCAN-C, which use speech
stimuli, can’t distinguish an auditory problem from a language problem. I made
similar arguments in a blog post written last year. Consider the task of doing a speech perception test in a
foreign language: if you don’t know the language very well, then you may fail
the test because you are poor at distinguishing unfamiliar speech sounds or
recognising specific words. This wouldn’t mean you had an auditory disorder.
- Filtered words: Repeat words that have been low-pass filtered, so they sound muffled
- Auditory figure-ground: Repeat words that are presented against background noise (multi-talker babble)
- Competing words: Repeat words that are presented simultaneously, one to each ear (dichotically)
- Competing sentences: Repeat sentences presented to one ear while ignoring those presented simultaneously to the other ear
A recent paper by
Loo et al (2012) provided concrete evidence for this concern. They compared
multilingual and monolingual children on performance on an APD battery. All
children were schooled in English, but a high proportion spoke another language
at home. The child’s language background
did not affect performance on non-linguistic APD tests, but had a significant
effect on most of the speech-based tests.
Results from the study were reported in 2010 and presented a challenge for the concept of
APD. Specifically, Moore et al concluded
that, when effect of task demands had been subtracted out, non-linguistic measures of auditory processing
“bore little relationship to measures of speech perception or to cognitive,
communication, and listening skills that are considered the hallmarks of APD in
children. This finding provides little support for the hypothesis that APD
involves impaired processing of basic sounds by the brain, as currently
embodied in definitions of APD.”
Overall, Moore et
al found that if we use auditory measures that are carefully controlled to
minimise effects of task demands and language ability, we find that they don’t
identify children about whom there is clinical concern. Nevertheless, children exist for whom there
is a clinical concern, insofar as the child reports difficulty in perceiving
speech in noise. So how on earth are we to proceed?
In the White
Paper, the BSA special interest group suggest that the focus should be on
developing standardized methods for identifying clinical characteristics of
APD, particularly through the use of parental questionnaires.
The experts who
responded to Moore and colleagues took a very different line. The specific points they raised varied, but
they were not happy with the idea of reliance on parental report as the basis
for APD diagnosis. In general, they
argued for more refined measures of auditory function. Jerger and Martin (USA)
expressed substantial agreement with Moore et al about the nature of the
problem confronting the APD concept. “There can be no doubt that attention,
memory, and language disorder are the elephants in the room. One can view them
either as confounds in traditional behavioral tests of an assumed sensory
disorder or, indeed, as key factors underlying the very nature of a ‘more
general neurodevelopmental delay’” . They rejected, however, the idea of
questionnaires for diagnosis, and suggested that methods such as
electroencephalography and brain imaging could be used to give more reliable
and valid measures of APD.
Dillon and
Cameron (Australia) queried the usefulness of a general term such as APD, when
the reality was that there may be many different types of auditory difficulty,
each requiring its own specific test. They described their own work on ‘spatial
listening disorder’, arguing that this did relate to clinical presentation.
The most critical
of Moore et al’s arguments were Bellis and colleagues (USA). They implied that
a good clinician can get around the confound between language and auditory
assessments: “Additional controls in cases in which the possible presence of a
linguistic or memory confound exists may include assessing performance in the
non-manipulated condition (e.g. monaural versus dichotic, nonfiltered versus
filtered, etc.) to ensure that performance deficits seen on CAPD tests are due
to the acoustic manipulations rather than to lack of familiarity with the
language and/or significantly reduced memory skills.” Furthermore, according to
Bellis et al, the fact that speech tasks don’t correlate with non-speech tasks
is all the more reason for using speech tasks in an assessment, because “in
some cases central auditory processing deficits may only be revealed using
speech tasks”.
Moore et al were
not swayed by these arguments. They argued first, that neurobiological
measures, such as electroencephalography, are no easier to interpret than
behavioural measures. I’d agree that it would be a mistake to assume such
measures are immune from top-down influences (cf. Bishop et al, 2012) and
reliability of measurement can be a serious problem (Bishop & Hardiman,2010). Moore et al were also critical of the idea that language factors can be
controlled for by within-task manipulations when speech tasks are used. This is
because the use of top-down information (e.g. using knowledge of vocabulary to
guess what a word is) becomes more important as a task gets harder, so a child
whose poor language has little impact on performance in an easy condition (e.g.
listening in quiet) may be much more affected when conditions get hard (e.g.
listening in noise). In addition, I would argue that the account by Bellis et
al implies that they know just how much allowance to make for a child’s
language level when giving a clinical interpretation of test findings. That is
a dangerous assumption in the absence of hard evidence from empirical studies.
So are we stuck
with the idea of diagnosing APD from parental questionnaires? Moore et al argue this is
preferable to other methods because it would at least reflect the child’s
symptoms, in a way that auditory tests don’t. I share the reservations of the
commentators about this, but for different reasons. To my mind this approach
would be justified only if we also changed the label that was used to refer to
these children. The research to date
suggests that children who report listening difficulties typically have
deficits in language, literacy, attention and/or social cognition (Dawes & Bishop, 2010; Ferguson et al, 2011). There’s not much evidence that these
problems are usually caused by low-level auditory disorder. It is therefore
misleading to diagnose children with APD on the basis of parental report alone,
as this label implies a primary auditory deficit.
In my view, we
should reserve APD as a term for
low-level auditory perceptual problems in children with normal hearing,
which are not secondary consequences of language or attentional deficits. The
problem is that we can’t make this diagnosis without more information about the
ways in which top-down influences impact on auditory measures, be they
behavioural or neurobiological. The population study by Moore et al (2010) made
a start on assessing how far non-linguistic auditory deficits related (or
failed to relate) to cognitive deficits and clinical symptoms in the general
population. The study by Loo et al (2012) adopts a novel approach to
understanding how language limitations can affect auditory test results, when
those limitations are due to the child’s language background, rather than any inherent
language disorder. The onus is now on those who advocate diagnosing APD on the
basis of existing tests to demonstrate that they are not only reliable but also
valid according to these kinds of criteria. Until they do so, the diagnosis of
APD will remain questionable.
P.S. 12th November 2012
Brief video by me on "Auditory processing disorder and language impairment" available here:
http://tinyurl.com/c2adbsy (with links to supporting slideshow and references)
ReferencesP.S. 12th November 2012
Brief video by me on "Auditory processing disorder and language impairment" available here:
http://tinyurl.com/c2adbsy (with links to supporting slideshow and references)
Loo, J., Bamiou, D., & Rosen, S. (2012). The Impacts of Language Background and Language-Related Disorders in Auditory Processing Assessment Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0068)
Moore, D., Rosen, S., Bamiou, D., Campbell, N., & Sirimanna, T. (2012). Evolving concepts of developmental auditory processing disorder (APD): A British Society of Audiology APD Special Interest Group ‘white paper’ International Journal of Audiology, 1-11 DOI: 10.3109/14992027.2012.723143
See also previous blogpost: "When commercial and clinical interests collide" (6th March 2011)
Thank you for this excellent post! From my vantage point (as a school psychology researcher in the US), it certainly seems that the evidentiary burden is on proponents of APD as a construct and a diagnostic label, and that burden hasn't come close to being met.
ReplyDeleteI think you tweeted about my general critique of APD some months ago (and I'm grateful for that):
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/pits.20595/abstract
Part of the problem seems to be that audiologists' primary assessment expertise is in assessing hearing sensitivity, a practice that involves very different technical measurement issues than the assessment of auditory processing does. As such, many audiologists are not very aware of psychometric theory, let alone the issues that can affect performance on cognitive tests. In particular, there's a common view that if a test (such as the SCAN) is developed and marketed for certain uses, it's like a piece of standard audiologic equipment - it's thought to have been vetted. When I was criticizing the SCAN at one convention (ASHA), more than one audiologist asked me, "Then how could they have published the test?"
Finally, I'm sympathetic to your definition of APD, but do we have much evidence that any children would meet it? That is, do we have clear evidence of a population of children who have normal peripheral hearing sensitivity and no general language or attention problems, but who nonetheless fail tests of low-level auditory processing?
(Of course, the proponents of APD have tried to have the issue both ways, by insisting on "owning" APD as an audiological disorder, but also insisting that APD is strongly related to attention and language issues.)
Thanks for your comment; glad to see the blog readership is extending across the Atlantic. I have indeed found your paper on this topic v. useful.
ReplyDeleteRe whether my definition of APD would be met, the way you put it would make the definition very hard to meet. And indeed, if the child didn't have any higher level problems, you might query whether you need to identify the problem anyhow! However, I'm not actually saying that the child should have no language or attentional problems - rather that poor performance on auditory tests should not be *explicable* in terms of such factors. We can't know if this is the case, unless and until we get good auditory tests that have been validated properly, either by showing that they are not influenced by the child's language/attentional level, OR by quantifying just how much performance is affected by language/attentional deficits, so it can be taken into account. I think that's a hard but not impossible goal. The Loo et al approach of assessing children with English as 2nd language is one way of quantifying language influences. And it's possible to test for attentional factors by, for instance, comparing auditory performance in ADHD children on and off medication (see Sutcliffe, P., Bishop, D., Houghton, S., & Taylor, M. 2006. Effect of attentional state on frequency discrimination: A comparison of children with ADHD on and off medication. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49, 1072-1084.)
Overall, I guess I am just being cautious because it seems to me feasible that you *could* have a language problem that resulted from lower-level auditory deficit. It's just that our current assessments don't let you unambiguously demonstrate that. So while I am skeptical, I don't want to prejudge matters until it's been properly tested.
I welcome this discussion. My team have attracted controversy in the UK for continuuuing to refer to a syndrome rather than specific disorder in adults. In our work we identified that illness perceptions (specifically beliefs around symptom conseqences) in adults determined help-seeking behaviour (and hence clinical labelling) to a greater extent than auditory factors.
ReplyDeletehttp://informahealthcare.com/doi/abs/10.3109/14992021003627892
What do you think about the notion that this is a multifaceted syndrome rather than a specific deficit or disorder?
Helen Pryce
Thanks Helen for your comment.
ReplyDeleteMy focus was solely on children. I don’t have experience with adult APD and it’s interesting to me to know if there is any continuity- i.e. do the adults you see have a history of APD in childhood? (Or conversely, has anyone followed up kids with an APD diagnosis to see if they present in adult ENT depts?) From very cursory acquaintance with the literature my impression is that these adults are often people who weren’t affected as children, but who start to complain of symptoms in adulthood, suggesting a different aetiology.
Obviously, one big issue with adults is that there is a range of neurological pathologies that can cause auditory symptoms, but I assume your focus in on people in whom known causes have been excluded.
I have to say I don’t like the label King-Kopetzky syndrome. This is for the same reason that I criticise medical-sounding labels for developmental disorders (see: http://deevybee.blogspot.co.uk/2010/12/whats-in-name.html). I think it can be misleading in implying that we are dealing with a disease entity with a known cause. Nevertheless, I am intrigued by the associations you have identified between APD complaints in adults and illness perception. And there is no doubt that a syndromic label gets a condition taken more seriously.
In children, I would stick with the conclusion we reached in Dawes and Bishop (2009), which was that APD should be seen as a symptom that could accompany a range of conditions, rather than a distinct syndrome. For adults, I’m less certain, but I agree that it makes sense to look beyond the auditory system for explanations.
i find your post quiet interesting. i would not know that quiet a number of people are following the AAA- ASHA- BSA issues on APD. while APD represent an auditory deficit, its diagnosis should not be based solely on poor response to auditory stimuli- though auditory evaluation is needed to distinguish it from hearing impairment. in whatever case, the most important comments should bother on managing the disorder, and also on relationship with co morbid disorders such as learning disabilities. i am a learning disabilities expert from nigeria
ReplyDeletei find your post quiet interesting. i would not know that quiet a number of people are following the AAA- ASHA- BSA issues on APD. while APD represent an auditory deficit, its diagnosis should not be based solely on poor response to auditory stimuli- though auditory evaluation is needed to distinguish it from hearing impairment. in whatever case, the most important comments should bother on managing the disorder, and also on relationship with co morbid disorders such as learning disabilities. i am a learning disabilities expert from nigeria
ReplyDeleteI can assure you APD doesn't just disappear when you become an "adult"! Oh no, that's just the start of your problems!
ReplyDeleteSlander, blackmail, harassment, etc... you can look forward to all kinds of cr@p from certain predatory individuals as they seek to fill the "void"... because you cannot tell anyone you have APD if you've never heard of it. You cannot claim to be a victim if you don't know what you have. You cannot counter accusations if you don't know what you have You cannot explain what you have if you don't know what it's called. And there's no point trying to explain it to others as it can't possibly exist if they've never heard of it, especially the audiologists!
You have to work harder and faster than others but you will still get the sack as you don't quite fit in. Almost no point getting a degree. APD trumps a degree every time!
So what if there is a false positive diagnosis of APD? Is the audio training going to cause any long term harm? Does the audio training even work? OK, so you find out it was a false positive, simply go back and diagnose something else, or simply say we don't know!
Plenty of other people will make false diagnoses about you. Your family will make false diagnoses. Your school "mates" will make false diagnoses. Your colleagues will make false diagnoses. Other "experts" will make false diagnoses, because they have never heard of APD., and you cannot refute any of it because you have never heard of APD either... not until 30 or so years later but by then it's far too late!
A very interesting read, thanks for the links. It would be interesting to see the comparison with Australia as well. Louise
ReplyDeleteIrrespective of what conclusions both organisations came to, it just shows we know very little in terms of the disorder. If both methods have been used and come up with the same results, then that is great, if not then that is where they need to reanalyses and find a result that will be more helpful to the children. Thank you for sharing this article and spreading awareness of this condition
ReplyDeleteEric | http://www.auditoryprocessingdisorderaustralia.com.au/auditory-processing-disorder
No need to go other site. Really this site is Awesome. I like it very much. you made nice post to get knowledge. I am really impressed. I want to see your post regularly. I hope you will make more post. Thank you so much.
ReplyDeleteemail processing system